The existence of protein complexes is supported by the following

The existence of protein complexes is supported by the following data: (1) Imaging experiments directly reveal assembly and vectorial Z-VAD-FMK ic50 transport of punctate fluorescent speckles containing these proteins (Figure S4). (2) Such particles are also seen natively by immunostaining (Figure S1). (3) Upon motor

inhibition, we saw clusters of stalled synapsin and CamKIIa particles clearly in axons (Figure 3B), suggesting that their mobility was interrupted by these manipulations. (4) Finally, biochemical experiments show that subsets of these cytosolic proteins are present in high-speed pellets from synaptosome-depleted (P100) as well as axon-enriched corpus callosum brain fractions that are detergent resistant, indicating the existence of higher order macromolecular structures within axons in vivo (Figure 5). We posit that the vast majority of these complexes transiently engage with motors (directly or indirectly) within axons, leading to a slow overall movement of the synapsin/CamKIIa population. Early nerve ligation/crushing

studies showed that synapsin was associated with vesicles accumulating proximally in ligated/crushed sites (Bööj et al., 1986). More detailed pulse-chase radiolabeling studies showed that while a small population LY2157299 concentration of newly synthesized synapsin (≈15%) departed the soma immediately afterwards, the vast majority (≈85%) was released from the cell body only after several days, and this pool moved much more slowly, at rates consistent with slow axonal transport (Baitinger

and Willard, 1987 and Petrucci et al., 1991). More recent studies have shown that in cultured neurons, synapsin is associated with mobile synaptic vesicular precursors (transport packets) probably conveyed in fast axonal transport (Ahmari et al., 2000). Synapsin is also an established component of synaptic vesicles (Takamori et al., 2006). Our data (Figure 4A) directly show that photoactivated somatic synapsin is transported into proximal axons both as punctate particles that are highly persistent and as a slow wave that departs the Idoxuridine soma with a transport behavior consistent with slow axonal transport. The persistent punctate synapsin particles colocalize with synaptophysin, a vesicular protein conveyed in fast axonal transport (Figure 4B). Collectively, the data indicate that a small fraction of newly synthesized synapsin is associated with vesicles and conveyed in fast axonal transport, while the remainder is conveyed in slow axonal transport with intricate particle kinetics. Though the biological basis for this bimodal transport behavior is unclear, it may have some evolutionary significance, in which cytosolic proteins in higher organisms may have acquired novel roles at synapses that require them to quickly localize to boutons, necessitating rapid transport in the fast component as well.

While most of the animals we observed exhibiting this pattern had

While most of the animals we observed exhibiting this pattern had cortical electrodes only, the animal shown in Figure 4D Akt inhibitor developed a hippocampal seizure lasting for almost 30 min while exhibiting cortical burst suppression. The animal was moribund during this event, raising the possibility that death in some animals might result from underlying nonconvulsive status epilepticus. The extent of CreERT2-mediated recombination varied among PTEN KO animals, so studies were undertaken to determine whether the percentage of granule cells in which PTEN was deleted correlated with epileptogenesis. Among the eight EEG-recorded PTEN KO mice (aged 3–7 months) for which

good histology was available (e.g., mice that survived and could

be perfusion-fixed), the percentage of dentate granule cells with no detectable PTEN immunoreactivity varied between <1% and 24%. Seizure activity was confirmed in seven of these mice, selleck chemicals llc with PTEN deletion measures of 9%–24%. No seizures were observed in the remaining animal that exhibited few PTEN KO granule cells (<1%). One limitation of the Gli1 promoter used to target hippocampal granule cell progenitors is that subventricular zone progenitors, which populate olfactory bulb via the rostral migratory stream, are also targeted. Cells produced by this pathway differentiate into inhibitory olfactory granule cells (≈95%; OGCs) or olfactory periglomerular cells (≈5%), the majority of which are also GABAergic ( Whitman and Greer, 2009). Although excess growth of inhibitory interneurons may, in principle, be less likely to promote epileptogenesis, it is a formal possibility. To explore this possibility, we first assessed the morphology of OGCs in olfactory bulb from wild-type and PTEN KO mice ( Figure 5). Initially, GFP expression was used to identify OGCs for morphological characterization in the olfactory bulb. Surprisingly,

in olfactory bulb, neither soma area (control, n = 5 [77 cells], 41.1 ± 2.2 μm2; all PTEN KO, n = 4 [44 cells], 47.2 ± 6.8; p = 0.381, t test) nor primary dendrite number (control, n = 5 [77 cells], 3.2 ± 0.2 dendrites/cell; PTEN KO, n = 4 [44 cells], 3.7 ± 0.6; p = 0.353, t test) differed between GFP-expressing OGCs in wild-type and PTEN KO mice. Given the robust morphological impact of PTEN deletion from hippocampal granule cells, we queried whether poor recombination efficiency among GFP expressing OGCs might account for this lack of effect. Analysis of PTEN/GFP double immunostaining revealed that only 44.6% ± 6.5% of GFP-expressing OGCs in PTEN KO mice were also immunonegative for PTEN. By contrast, 84% ± 6.1% of GFP-expressing dentate granule cells in PTEN KO mice were PTEN immunonegative (p = 0.007, t test). These data suggest that cre recombinase is more effective at inducing GFP expression and deleting PTEN from hippocampal granule cells relative to OGCs, although the mechanism of this phenomenon is not clear.

The outstanding question of whether these changes are due to diff

The outstanding question of whether these changes are due to differential inputs or due to intrinsic properties of the neurons remains unanswered, as does the extent to which these mechanisms are involved in experience-dependent learning such as drug seeking. It will be important to measure plasticity in response to more behaviorally relevant protocols that emulate learning in response to reward and aversion, perhaps incorporating optogenetic or other approaches

to more clearly isolate particular inputs altered by stimuli. Further characterization of changes in the AMPA to NMDA ratio in Ih− DA neurons is required to determine if these cells exhibit a change in the subunit composition of AMPA receptors (e.g., a switch to calcium-permeable GluA2-lacking receptors) that has been linked to drug-induced Epacadostat in vitro behavioral sensitization and conditioned place preference (Lüscher and Malenka, 2011). A related issue is that while altered DA neurotransmission in the striatum and NAc is strongly implicated http://www.selleckchem.com/products/pd-0332991-palbociclib-isethionate.html in various aspects of drug dependence, it is less clear if an altered AMPA to NMDA ratio as a form of plasticity plays a role. If the AMPA receptors are maximally induced by exposure to an addictive drug, would this occlude reward-related

learning for the duration? It may be that the more complex alterations at corticostriatal synapses induced by these drugs lead to very long-term habits. The finding that a drug that elevates DA transmission and is associated with reward or addiction (and pain, as a model of aversive stimuli) could involve analogous synaptic plasticity at different DA cells certainly will motivate new investigations. The excitatory input to the DA cells is extensive and involves glutamatergic afferents from the prefrontal cortex, superior colliculus, pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus, lateral dorsal tegmental nucleus, subthalamic nucleus, and additional areas (Sesack and Grace, SB-3CT 2010), and any of these could be responsible for differential responses of the VTA neurons. Moreover, there are multiple

inhibitory and modulatory inputs and collaterals, and appropriate disinhibition or frequency-dependent filtering could play the key role in determining which inputs mediate this diverse plasticity. Using anatomically rigorous techniques, Lammel et al. have now provided us a far more detailed roadmap of the VTA. Future studies of these neurons will need to take into account more precisely which DA neurons are examined, including whether a neuron expresses TH+ and expresses Ih, with some idea of where the projections lie. As it is now relatively clear that some VTA DA neurons use glutamate as a cotransmitter (Hnasko et al., 2010), precisely which of them do so, and why? Most promisingly, these findings suggest new means to determine more precisely which synapses regulate behavior.

However, it is likely that in the intact animal, there is a dynam

However, it is likely that in the intact animal, there is a dynamic interdependency between goal-directed and habitual systems and that control is likely Akt inhibitor to emerge simultaneously and competitively (Wassum et al., 2009). If habit and goal-directed processes indeed act concurrently, then this invites questions regarding what precisely are the factors that influence the integration and competition between the two systems. We return to these issues below. It is also worth noting here that although

goal-directed or response-outcome learning has a strong declarative flavor, it is conceptually distinct from a hippocampal-dependent stimulus-stimulus form of learning. There are some alluring parallels with this account of the evolution from goal-directed to habitual responding. One is the transfer of control of a simple spatial behavior (turning in a “plus” maze) from a hippocampal-dependent, allocentric, reference frame to a striatum-dependent, egocentric one (Packard and McGaugh, 1996). Similar double dissociations arise from reversible lesions in these two regions at different time points, for example early or late, during learning.

The other parallel is with the transfer over the course of experience from allocentric to egocentric reference frames of a sequence of manual button presses (Hikosaka et al., 1999), although this was proposed to depend on two separate cortical regions that both interact with the basal ganglia. Recent lesion studies have examined more sophisticated representational issues, Gefitinib for Carnitine palmitoyltransferase II instance, comparing the sort of stimulus-response associations that underpin habits to a hierarchical association scheme in which the presence of a certain stimulus implies that a response leads to a particular outcome (Bradfield and Balleine, 2013). Control apparently based on the latter representation

is compromised by lesions to posterior dorsomedial striatum, whereas in complex circumstances, lesions to dorsolateral striatal actually enhanced learning, suggesting that a form of competition might be at work. The rich backdrop of animal experiments has inspired a collection of studies that address the architecture of human instrumental control, often employing straightforward adaptations of successful animal paradigms as well as seeking and exploiting homologies (Balleine and O’Doherty, 2010 and Haber and Knutson, 2010). Many of these have involved the use of fMRI in order to investigate the neural representation of the value of stimuli and actions to see whether or not they are affected by devaluation. We consider two studies of particular interest in this context that respectively target goal-directed and habitual choice (Valentin et al., 2007 and Tricomi et al., 2009). Valentin and colleagues trained human subjects on a task in which two different instrumental actions resulted in two distinct food reward outcomes (Valentin et al., 2007).

g , Hatsopoulos and Donoghue, 2009) not only to compensate for th

g., Hatsopoulos and Donoghue, 2009) not only to compensate for the deficits and retrain lesioned brain and bodies, but also, once noninvasive techniques are further developed, to augment the capability of intact brains. The potential ethical and social implications of such capabilities should not escape our notice. We are grateful to present and past Panobinostat molecular weight students for many discussions about these issues. Y.D.’s research is supported by the I-CORE Program of the Planning and Budgeting Committee and The Israel Science Foundation (grant 51/11). R.G.M.M.’s research is supported by the European

Research Council. “
“Vertebrate brains are among the most sophisticated scalable architectures in nature. Scalability refers to a property that allows the system to grow and perform ABT-737 price the same desired computations, often with increased efficacy. In scalable systems, certain aspects of the system must be constrained if the same computational goals are to be achieved in the face of increasing organismal complexity. In this essay, we submit that temporal organization of neuronal activity, represented

by the system of rhythms, is one of the fundamental constraints in scaling brain size. When Neuron got its start 25 years ago, the study of neuronal oscillations was largely confined to clinical electroencephalography, invertebrate physiology, sleep research, and a few laboratories devoted to the study of the relationships between specific local-field-potential rhythms and behavior or perceptual processes. Today, the study of brain rhythms is an intertwined part of systems neuroscience Levetiracetam and among its fastest growing fields. This shift is largely due to the recognition that the multifarious

rhythms of the brain form a hierarchical system that offers a syntactical structure for the spike traffic within and across circuits at multiple time scales. The constellation of network rhythms is characteristic of individual brains, and their alterations invariably lead to mental and neurological disease. In today’s world of the “connectome,” it is worth reiterating that network oscillations are among the most conservatively preserved phenotypes in mammalian evolution. What are the structural and physiological solutions that allow the preservation of the syntactical rules of spike communication in the face of rapidly growing brain size? Answering this question is among the most critical in neuroscience and amounts to an understanding of the neuronal “code. Brains, small and large, are predictive devices that exploit regularity and recurrence as a fundamental property of the surrounding world and apply effective heuristics acquired through phylogenetic and individual experience for problem solving. The brain’s ability to work both as a subsumption and as a prediction device relies on a set of complex properties, including self-organized information retention and local-global integration.

contortus infection, in comparison with sheep breeds of European

contortus infection, in comparison with sheep breeds of European origin also raised in Brazil ( Rocha et al., 2005, Bricarello

et al., 2005, Costa et al., 2007 and Amarante et al., 2009). CHIR-99021 in vivo However, Amarante et al. (2004) compared Santa Ines, Ile de France and Suffolk lambs that were naturally infected by gastrointestinal nematodes and observed a higher resistance to H. contortus and Oesophagostomum columbianum infections, but all three breeds had similar T. colubriformis worm burdens. Similar results were obtained in the comparison between Florida Native (resistant) with Rambouillet (susceptible) lambs ( Amarante et al., 1999), as well as in the comparison of Gulf Coast Native (resistant) with Suffolk (susceptible) lambs ( Bahirathan

et al., 1996). These results indicate that, in those “resistant breeds”, there are differences in the host–parasite interaction regarding the different nematode species, i.e., the immune response possibly acts with higher efficiency against Haemonchus spp. infection than against Trichostrongylus CH5424802 molecular weight spp. The losses generated by infection with T. colubriformis reinforce the importance of research involving this neglected nematode in South America, coupled with the lack of studies on this parasite in hair sheep was the motivation for the present experiment, which was carried out to evaluate the immune response and the impact of T. colubriformis artificial infections on the performance of Santa Ines growing lambs. Thirty lambs were raised indoors with their mothers since birth and were weaned at 60 days of age. After that they were transferred to individual pens with a concrete floor that was washed with water under pressure every two days. During an initial two week-period of adaptation, the animals only were vaccinated against clostridioses (Sintoxan Polivalente®–Merial S/A, Brazil) and fecal examinations

were carried out that showed that 15 lambs were shedding trichostrongylid eggs with a maximum of 200 eggs per gram (EPG). One of the animals also passed 200 EPG of Strongyloides spp. and Eimeria spp. oocysts were also sporadically observed in some animals, however, in small quantities. Larvae from Haemonchus spp. (78%), Trichostrongylus spp. (12%) and Cooperia spp. (10%) were found in fecal cultures from these lambs. For this reason, all lambs were orally treated with levamisole phosphate (Ripercol L 150 F® – Fort Dodge, Brazil) and albendazole (Valbazen 10 Cobalto® – Pfizer, Brazil) for three consecutive days. Subsequently, the animals also received trichlorfon (Neguvon® – Bayer, Brazil) for an additional three days. Seven days later, fecal examinations were repeated and three animals that were still shedding eggs in faeces were subjected to a new series of treatment with the same anthelmintics. Nematode trichostrongylid eggs were not detected in the fecal tests after this procedure.

, 2006) Second, Mek1,2\hGFAP conditional nulls that survive thro

, 2006). Second, Mek1,2\hGFAP conditional nulls that survive through the first postnatal week display a dorsal cortex that is almost completely devoid of astrocytes and exhibit a major neurodegeneration phenotype. Although both neurons and glia lack MEK in these mice, results from neuron-specific Mek-deleted mice suggest that neurons can survive into adulthood in the absence of MEK (data not shown), indicating the degeneration in Mek1,2\hGFAP dorsal cortices is probably due to the lack of glial support. A similar situation holds in the periphery where MEK/ERK signaling is required for Schwann cell development and neurons deprived of Schwann cell

support die massively during embryonic development ( Newbern et al., 2011). Finally, subcortical dopamine neuron survival GSK-3 assay has also been shown to be critically dependent on the astrocyte-derived trophic factors-conserved dopamine neurotrophic factor (CDNF) and mesencephalic astrocyte-derived neurotrophic factor (MANF) ( Lindholm et al., 2007; Petrova et al., 2003). The nature of glial-derived survival signals for cortical neurons remains to be determined and should be a rich area for future investigation. It is important to note that postnatal regulation is critical to establishing the number this website of astrocytes and oligodendroglia in the

mature CNS. It has long been known that proliferation of OPCs postnatally is regulated by PDGF (Fruttiger et al., 1999). Very recently it has been demonstrated that mature-appearing astrocytes in upper cortical layers also proliferate in the postnatal period (Ge et al., 2012). Further recent studies demonstrate Levetiracetam that oligodendrocyte proliferation in spinal cord is partially under ERK/MAPK control (Newbern et al., 2011) and that constitutively active B-Raf can drive proliferation of spinal cord astrocyte precursors (Tien et al., 2012). These results, in combination with our results showing expansion of astrocytes in mice expressing caMek1, all strongly suggest that postnatal stages of glial development may also be regulated by MEK/ERK/MAPK signaling. Lastly, we note that astrocytes are now known to play critical roles in synapse

formation, elimination, and function (Allen and Barres, 2005; Christopherson et al., 2005; Stevens et al., 2007). However, the consequences of increasing astrocyte number for cortical neuronal physiology and behavior are unknown. Our MEK hyperactivation model may provide a unique approach to study the effects of changing the glia/neuron ratio on synapse formation and neuronal activity. Such studies may facilitate our understanding of the role of glia in the cognitive abnormalities observed in CFC syndrome patients. The Mek1f/f, Mek2−/−, Erk1-/, Erk2f/f, and CAG-loxpSTOPloxp-Mek1S218E,S222E (caMek1) mouse lines and associated genotyping procedures have been previously described ( Krenz et al., 2008; Newbern et al., 2008), and see Supplemental Experimental Procedures.

Similarly, the activity of an orthologous spinal microcircuit may

Similarly, the activity of an orthologous spinal microcircuit may be responsible for the grasp reflex in the human infant. Human fetuses develop a grasp reflex in the first trimester (Hooker, 1938) that persists in the postnatal period for 2−6 months (Halverson, 1937; Pollack, 1960). Reflexive grasping is not normally seen in adult humans, most likely because

find more higher systems regulate this microcircuit, which may also be involved in feed-forward control of hand function (see Rushworth and Denny-Brown, 1959). Presumably, these reflexes disappear because of the development of the brain and descending systems. Grasp reflexes emerge in adults with structural brain (Walshe and Hunt, 1936) and neurodegenerative diseases and their pathological reemergence can be quite disabling for both hand (Mestre and Lang, 2010) and foot function (Paulson and Gottlieb, 1968). In addition, the opposite effect—a loss of normal control of hand grasp, resulting, for example, from spinal cord injury—is significantly disabling (Anderson, 2004). Understanding dI3 INs and their control will aid in the development of microcircuit-targeted therapies to improve hand dysfunction in disease or following injury. Perifosine mw Expression of YFP driven

by the promoter for the homeodomain transcription factor Isl1 was obtained in double transgenic offspring of Isl1+/Cre and Thy1-lox-stop-lox-YFP mice. The following strains of mice were generously donated and used in this study: Thy1-lox-stop-lox-YFP mice (from J. Sanes) and Thy1-lox-stop-lox-mGFP (from S. Arber). Conditional knockout of vGluT2 Farnesyltransferase in Isl1-expressing neurons (dI3OFF) was accomplished by crossing

Isl1+/Cre mice with a strain of mice bearing a conditional allele of the Slc17a6 (vGluT2) gene where exon 2 of the gene was flanked by loxP sequences (vGluT2flox/flox; Figure 5A). This resulted in Cre-mediated excision of exon 2 of the vGluT2 gene in Isl1-expressing neurons ( Hnasko et al., 2010). All animal procedures were approved by the University Committee on Laboratory Animals of Dalhousie University and conform to the guidelines put forth by the Canadian Council for Animal Care. Additional methodological details can be found in Supplemental Information. Sagittal hemicords were prepared from Isl1-YFP or dI3OFF postnatal (P5–P16) mice. After anesthesia was administered by an injection of a mixture of xylazine and ketamine, mice were decapitated, and spinal cords were isolated by vertebrectomy in room temperature recording artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF) (NaCl, 127 mM; KCl, 3 mM; NaH2PO4, 1.2 mM; MgCl2, 1 mM; CaCl2, 2 mM; NaHCO3, 26 mM; D-glucose, 10 mM). Ventral and dorsal roots were dissected as distally as possible. Cords were hemisected by a midline longitudinal incision, incubated for 45–60 min in 37°C recording ACSF, and equilibrated in room temperature recording ACSF for at least 30 min.

However, processes other than growth may contribute to changing c

However, processes other than growth may contribute to changing cell size or number. For example, Alectinib ic50 learning may enhance the survival of recently created new neurons (Zhao et al., 2008). Hence, it is possible that a decreased MD in the dentate gyrus reflects a slowing of the cell death process in the learning group while cell pruning continued at a higher rate in the control groups. The histological results provide important evidence about what cellular changes accompany the detected MRI effects, but they cannot directly demonstrate whether any or all of these particular cellular changes

drive the observed MD change. Future studies using pharmacological or genetic manipulations could test more directly the relationships between specific cellular changes and MRI effects. The rapid and perhaps transient nature of these learning-related changes provides a further challenge. In vivo methodologies will be useful to fully understand how neural tissue changes in the minutes and hours after learning. Thus, molecular and optical imaging are perhaps most suited to understand how these compartments change in the living organism. The present work, along with previous studies (Blumenfeld-Katzir et al., 2011 and Lerch et al., 2011) combining imaging and histology, provides valuable selleck screening library insights into the types of structural

changes that can be detected on different timescales with noninvasive MRI. For instance, 5 days of training in the water maze task increased the volume of the hippocampus, as measured with MRI, and produced a correlated increase in GAP-43, a marker for neuronal process remodeling (Lerch et al., 2011). In another study using 5 days of training with the same task, changes in diffusion MRI parameters were related to increases in GFAP, synaptophysin, and myelin basic protein (MBP) (Blumenfeld-Katzir et al., 2011).

Chlormezanone The time frame of these studies allows for slower remodeling mechanisms like dendritric sprouting or gliogenesis to occur (Figure 1). Such mechanisms could contribute to the structural brain changes detected using MRI in humans with long-term learning (Draganski et al., 2004 and Scholz et al., 2009). Sagi and colleagues′ results provide us with an important reminder that the brain is an extremely dynamic structure. This study used a focused period of video game playing, but presumably many of the learning experiences we undergo throughout our lives produce similar effects in task-relevant regions of our brains. The findings therefore have more general implications for human neuroimaging. Many studies that employ the standard imaging methods used here assume that human brain structure is relatively static, at least on short timescales.

For example, in the North of England and Scotland average tempera

For example, in the North of England and Scotland average temperatures during the active growing season of the spring crop remain below 15 °C which is a more favourable

environment for growth and infection by Microdochium species ( Parry et al., 1995 and Xu et al., 2008). In contrast, F. poae requires dry and warm conditions of around 25 °C for optimum growth ( Parry et al., 1995 and Xu et al., 2008). F. graminearum infection is more often associated with wet and warm conditions during anthesis, whereas Selleck Epigenetic inhibitor F. culmorum, F. avenaceum and F. tricinctum require wet, humid and cool environmental conditions ( Xu et al., 2008). There were only small differences between the barley cultivars included in our studies with respect to the amounts of pathogen DNA present. The exception was cv Shuffle which had significantly lower amounts of total fungal DNA, irrespective of region, compared with the other elite cultivars such as Concerto, Forensic, Optic, Westminster (P = 0.042). This indicates that current commercially available cultivars, at least in the UK, are of similar susceptibility to Fusarium infection. Only a few sources of FHB resistance are known in barley, however, the level of resistance, even in these, is at best moderate ( Bai and Shaner, 2004). Mycotoxin analysis of the UK barley samples revealed that the predominant mycotoxins were DON followed by NIV and ZON and lastly by HT-2 and T-2 at low concentrations. PLX4032 nmr In 2010

and 2011 a large number of samples were analysed to obtain a representative overview of the natural mycotoxin contamination in English and Scottish fields and these were all found to be below the legislative limits of Fusarium related mycotoxins. In contrast to HT-2 and T-2, DON and NIV were found in significantly higher concentrations in 2011 than in 2010. The sum of HT-2 and T-2 found in the barley samples from 2010 was significantly associated with DNA of F. langsethiae. Besides F. langsethiae, F. sporotrichioides is also

known to produce HT-2 and T-2 ( Thrane et al., 2004). However in the UK, previous studies in oats have shown a strong relationship between combined HT-2 and T-2 levels and DNA amounts of F. langsethiae ( Edwards et al., 2012), whereas in Europe three different species, F. langsethiae, Montelukast Sodium F. sporotrichoides or Fusarium sibiricum, are associated with HT-2 and T-2 ( Fredlund et al., 2010, Yli-Mattila et al., 2008 and Yli-Mattila et al., 2009). The barley samples were analysed for F. sporotrichioides DNA with primers known to cross-react with F. sibiricum ( Yli-Mattila et al., 2011) but failed to detect the DNA of either species or to isolate any of these species from barley grain. Thus, the evidence suggests that in the UK, contamination with HT-2 and T-2 in both oats and barley is predominantly associated with F. langsethiae. Isolates of F. graminearum, F. culmorum and F. poae are able to produce NIV; in the present study only F. poae correlated strongly (R2 = 0.